Selectează o Pagină

(Credits for the conference art go to Alina Veronica Petre, See: https://alinapetre.myportfolio.com)

Jabberwocky Words in Linguistics:
 Making Sense through Nonsense.  Making Sense of Nonsense.

 

Meeting Description:

The online workshop “Jabberwocky Words In Linguistics Workshop”  will take place online:

11-12 February 2022

Organizers:  Adina Camelia Bleotu, University of Bucharest

Deborah Foucault Etheridge, University of Massachusetts Amherst

Invited speakers (in alphabetical order):
Eve Clark (Stanford University)
Jennifer Culbertson (University of Edinburgh) and Alexander Martin (Université de Paris)
Anouk Dieuleveut (LLF, Université de Paris), Ailís Cournane (New York University) and Valentine Hacquard (University of Maryland)
Brian Dillon (University of Massachusetts Amherst) and Jon Burnsky (University of Massachusetts Amherst)
Heidi Harley (University of Arizona)
Jeff Lidz (University of Maryland)
Letitia Naigles (University of Connecticut)
Emma Nguyen (University of California, Irvine)
Gillian Ramchand (UiT The Arctic University of Norway)
Tom Roeper (University of Massachusetts Amherst)
Kristen Syrett (Rutgers University)
Lyn Tieu (Western Sydney University)

Schedule of Event: Click Here

Access link: https://umass-amherst.zoom.us/j/3510456920

About this workshop

“If I had a world of my own, everything would be nonsense. Nothing would be what it is because everything would be what it isn’t.  And contrary wise, what is, it wouldn’t be. And what it wouldn’t be, it would. You see?”

-Lewis Carroll, Alice in Wonderland

“Don’t for heaven’s sake, be afraid of talking nonsense! But you must pay attention to your nonsense.”

-Ludwig Wittgenstein, Culture and Value

The current workshop aims at bringing into focus research which sheds light on linguistic structures/phenomena by means of non-existent words. The workshop welcomes research making use of possible nonce words, i.e., words which happen not to be part of the current language but could potentially exist (such as blick in The man blicked yesterday). Previous experimental work has used nonce words to investigate how children acquire language. In particular, nonce words have been employed to investigate the syntactic bootstrapping hypothesis, according to which children use syntactic cues to get to the meaning of words. The hypothesis originates as early as 1957, when Roger Brown showed experimentally that preschool-aged children could use their knowledge of different parts of speech to distinguish the meaning of nonsense words in English (Do you see any/ a sib?, What is sibbing?). Later, Gleitman (1990) coined the term syntactic bootstrapping, and further on stressed the importance of syntactic cues in acquisition. Interesting experimental work further ensued (Naigles, 1990; Soja, 1992; Höhle et al., 2004; Cristophe et al., 2008; Syrrett et. al., 2010; Yuan & Fisher, 2012; Jin & Fisher, 2014; He & Lidz, 2017; Cao & Lewis, 2021; Huang et al., 2021; a.o.): to elaborate on one influential work, Naigles (1990) showed by means of a (video-based) eye-tracking paradigm that 2-year-olds who hear The duck is kradding the rabbit interpreted kradding as the act of the duck pushing on the rabbit, whereas 2-year-olds who hear The duck and the rabbit are kradding interpret kradding as the act of both animals doing something (arm waving). The nonce paradigm has the advantage of eliminating lexical biases created by existent words in the lexicon, and, instead, isolating the issue of interest. In addition to their relevance for the acquisition of (lexical) semantics, nonce words have also been employed to investigate the acquisition of morphology. The famous Wug Test, created by Jean Berko Gleason in 1958 and replicated multiple times, used nonce words to explore children’s acquisition of plural morphology (one wug-two wugs), possessives (wug’s, wugs’) and verbal morphology (He zibs). We are interested in recent experimental work which uses nonce words as a tool to investigate the acquisition of lexical and functional items.

Apart from employing novel words within an existing language, another method which has become a useful tool in exploring linguistic universals is artificial language learning: participants have to learn a novel artificial grammar, and their perfomance can give useful information about innate linguistic biases (Culbertson, Smolensky, & Legendre, 2012; Ettlinger, Bradlow, & Wong, 2014; Finley & Badecker, 2009; a.o.). For instance, Culbertson et al. (2012, 2015, 2017, 2020) have shown that child and adult learners are biased in favor of harmonic word patterns (either prenominal harmonic orders like Adj N, Num N or postnominal harmonic orders like N Adj, N Num), and that this bias holds even when learner’s native language is non-harmonic (like French or Hebrew, which are N Adj, Num N). We thus also welcome papers making use of artificial language learning to investigate linguistic principles.

Other areas of interest for the workshop involve novel words which are used spontaneously by certain individual speakers such as novel denominal verbs like to giraffe (Clark & Clark 1979; Hale & Keyser 2002; Harley 2005), novel compound coinages like pony-kid or angel-cake (Clark 1993) or impossible words which can simply not exist in the language (such as *faller or *dier, for instance), given certain structural constraints which limit the domain of creativity (Roeper, 1987).

We welcome experimental papers employing nonce words within various methodologies (act-out tasks, truth value judgment tasks, eye-tracking, story-telling, artificial language learning, a.o.) to investigate certain linguistic phenomena/structures (first and second language acquisition, language processing, online and offline methodological issues, a.o.). We also welcome corpus and theoretical work which provides new insight into how novel words are created and what this can tell us about the linguistic mechanisms people make use of.

We invite abstracts for 30-minute talks (with a 10-minute discussion included). Abstracts should be no longer than 500 words in a font size no less than 12pt, with an additional page including examples, figures and references. Abstracts should be anonymous. Contact details (author’s name and affiliation) and the title of the presentation should be included in the accompanying email.

Please send your abstract (PDF format) to cameliableotu@gmail.com and dfoucaulteth@umass.edu

Important Dates:

Deadline for abstract submission: January 3, 2022 (extended to 20th)

Notification of acceptance: January 20, 2022 (extended to 31st)

Workshop: February 11-12, 2022